The question I wanted to know is whether any of these sorts of communications actually has any impact on the people for whom it is intended.
In the most recent issue of my monthly Food Demand Survey (FooDS), we sought to address this issue. 1,017 respondents were randomly allocated to one of three information groups or treatments. In the first no-info group, respondents were simply told, “About 90% of feedlot cattle are given added growth hormones to improve the rate of growth.” And then, respondents were asked, “How concerned are you about the use of growth hormones in beef production?”
For the second group text-only group, written text was added to convey relative risks of hormone use. Prior to being asked level of concern, subjects were told, “About 90% of feedlot cattle are given added growth hormones to improve the rate of growth. The added hormones add about 3 extra nanograms (a billionth of a gram) to a 3 oz serving of beef. For comparison purposes, the amount of estrogen that naturally occurs in 3 oz of the following foods is: potatoes (225 nanograms), peas (340 nanograms), cabbage (2,000 nanograms), soybean oil (170,000 nanograms).”
Finally, the third visual+text group was given the same written text but was also shown the above visual illustration using M&Ms allocated to different jars.
Participants in all three groups answered with their level of concern on a five-point scale (1 = very unconcerned; 5=very concerned).
Information on relative risks caused a small but statistically significant reduction in the level of concern. The mean levels of concern, on the 5-point scale, were 3.93, 3.71, and 3.66 for the no-info, text-only, and text+visual information groups.
Without any information on relative risks, over 71% of respondents indicated that they were either concerned or very concerned. Textual information reduced that frequency to 66%, and visual+text information further reduced the percentage of concerned respondents to 63.6%.